Reports of
individual who appeared insensitive to pain from birth onwards have a long
history but it was not until the 1930’s that this condition attracted medical
attention.
The 1st case
was reported by Dr. Dearborn in 1932 — he described his patient who acts as a
human pincushion in a circus and crucifixion had to be called off when a woman
in the audience fainted after a spike was driven through his one hand.
Initially, various
terms were used to describe these individuals including ‘Congenital General
Pure Analgesia’ (Dearborn, 1932), ‘Congenital Universal Insensitiveness to
Pain’ (Ford & Wilkinsons, 1938), ‘Congenital Universal Indifference to
Pain’ (Winkelmann et al., 1962). As these labels show, the phenomenon
encompasses diverse abnormal responses to pain. Some patients have an absence
of responses to injury, abnormal automatic responses to painful stimuli and
difficulties in distinguishing various types of stimuli, whereas others exhibit
lack of responsiveness to the stimuli but retain the ability to identify
stimulus presence and mobility.
Over time 2 terms predominantly
describe these individuals — ‘Congenital Insensitivity to Pain’ (McMurray 1950)
and ‘Congenital Indifference to Pain’ (Jewesbury, 1970). Although the terms
were often used interchangeably, only in recent years they have acquired
distinct meanings and careful authors now to use them to distinguish two groups
of individuals (Jewesbury, 1970; Landrieu et al., 1990).
Patients with congenital
insensitivity pain seem not to perceive sensations of pain i.e. they have
remarkably impaired ability to perceive the type, intensity and quality of
painful stimuli. On the other hand, those with congenital indifference to pain,
however, painful stimuli are perceived but there is an absence of the affective
response to pain, rather than a lack of signal transmission. These individual
reports experiencing sensations of pain but exhibit no aversion to or
withdrawal from painful stimuli.
No comments:
Post a Comment